Child Routines at Home and Social Competence of Preschool Children’s
Yosef Bisetegn Mihiretu1*, MaylafHiruy Alemayehu2
1 Early Childhood Care and Development Specialist, Information Network Security Administration (INSA) Ethiopia.
2 Social Psychologist Researchers, Information Network Security Administration, Ethiopia.
*Corresponding Author
Yosef Bisetegn Mihiretu,
Early Childhood Care and Development Specialist, Information Network Security Administration (INSA) Ethiopia.
E-mail: Yosefbisetegn@gmail.com
Received: June 13, 2022; Accepted: October 14, 2022; Published: October 19, 2022
Citation: Yosef Bisetegn Mihiretu, Maylaf Hiruy Alemayehu. Child Routines at Home and Social Competence of Preschool Children’s Int J Behav Res Psychol. 2022;10(02):293-299.
Copyright: Yosef Bisetegn Mihiretu© 2022. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Abstract
Popular parenting literature has proposes that the importance of establishing predictable and flexible child routines at home for preschool children’s. Experts also emphasize that child routines at home are serve as a paramount practice in healthy and holistic child development [7]. Using a sample of 70 preschool-aged children’sin Addis Ababa, Ethiopian, the current study was examined is there child routines at home correlated with social competence of preschool children’s. This study examined gender difference of children’s social competence by using teacher reported (social competence teacher rating scale), and gender difference of child routines at home for preschool children by using parent reported of child routine questionnaire. The instrument demonstrated reliability and validity. Furthermore, the findings showed that child routines at home in discipline, daily living, positive attention, educationalor social routines, and hygiene related routines were all positively correlated to social competence domains (play skills, emotion regulation, and communication skills and negatively correlated with pro-social behaviors. As a result of the study it was found that there wasn’t any statistically significant difference between the children’s gender in their social competence and child routines scale total scores and sub scale scores. The current study provided initial evidence on the role of child routines at home to promoting social competence among sample of preschool children in Addis Ababa, Ethiopian.
2.Keywords
3.Introduction
4.Materials and Methods
5.Results
6.Discussion
7.Funding
8.References
Keywords
Child Routines; Preschool; And Social Competence.
Introduction
Popular parenting literature has often emphasized the importance
of establishing predictable child routines at home during early
childhood. Although, emerging empirical research has provided
some evidence for the importance of child routines at home [3,
5], understanding of the role that child routines play in the development
of preschool children is still limited [5], particularly
preschool children’s in Ethiopia. The current study focused on
Ethiopian parents with preschool-aged children. In Ethiopia, the
importance of setting child routines at home and its relationship
with social competence of preschool children’s has not been forwardedfor
parents, teachers and significant other, rather it is likely
hindered by the co-parenting roles commonly taken by extended
family members such as grandparents and caregivers.
Therefore, it is important to establish whether child routines at
home are important to domains of preschool children’s in Ethiopia,
and if so, how consistency in child routines at home may
be correlated with social competence preschool children’s. In the
world, child routines at home have attracted researchers’ interest
since the 1950s, and they both concern repeated practices that
involve two or more family members [7]. In the current study,
we focus on child routines at home, which can be viewed as child
routines pertaining to the parents.
.
Sytsma, Kelley, and Wymer (2001) [16] defined child routines as
“observable, and repetitive behaviors which directly involve the
child and at least one adult acting in an interactive or facilitator
role, and which occur with predictable regularity in the daily and/
or weekly life of the child. Common child routines at home for
preschool children include activities pertaining to play, manipulating
materials, painting, story telling, reading, mealtimes, bedtime
and others [5].
Some researchers have also conceptualized parents’ consistency
in reinforcing discipline and other behaviors as a component of
child routines at home [17, 18]. Many researchers proposes that
child routines at home provide children with a sense of predictability,
stability, and feelings of security, which fosters their social and other domains of development [1, 5, 8]. Sytsma et al. (2001)
[17] proposed a behavioral theory contending that child routines
at home are serving as a setting behavior for children’s compliance
by providing them with consistent and predictable environmental
cues to perform expected behaviors throughout the day. Consistent
with this theory, it has been found that regular child routines
at home contribute to a wide range of socialand academic competence
of preschool children.
For instance, consistent child routines at home were correlatedwith
behavioral difficulties among preschool-aged children [3].
Attention has also been paid to the effect of child routines at
home in the development of preschool children, as development
in the early years often has long-lasting implications for children’s
latersocial and other developmental aspects [4]. Ferretti and Bub
(2014) [5] found that family routines involving the child were
positively associated with children’s self-regulation, and early routines
also contributed to their cognitive abilities. Bater and Jordan
(2017) [1] reported that preschoolers with more consistent routines
at home demonstrated a better social competence and fewer
externalizing challenges.
Despite the extensive studies conducted elsewhere in relation to
child routines at home for preschool children’s (Garces, Thomas,
& Currie, 2002; Gormley, Gayer, Phillips, & Dawson, 2005; Howes
et al., 2008), Powell et al., 2010), there is no study conducted in
Ethiopia on this issue.In Ethiopia there nosources of investigate
in preschool children’s routines, most existing measures have only
focused challenges, practices, and qualities of early education in
the preschools, and those intended to investigate child routines
at home specific to preschool children’s are limited in Ethiopia.
The term social competence refers to effectiveness of successfully
and appropriately differentiates and carries out their interpersonal
goals [11] and socially competent children have been
defined as those who participate in satisfying interactions and
activities with parents, caregivers, and peers [12]. And also, social
competence is a major aspect of child development related to
peers acceptance, communication skill, play skill, pro-social behavior,
emotional regulation and other skills as established in preschool
years for readiness purpose in interpersonal relationships
and social adjustment. Children who are socially competent, cooperative,
and friendly are likely to do well in social and academic
areas and demonstrate a higher psychological resilience. Similarly,
studies indicate that, preschool children social competence is an
indicator of effectiveness and appropriateness in child interaction
and relationships with parents, caregivers, and age mates [2].
Daily relationships of the child with family such as, talking, playing
game, have the characteristics of routines at home relating to
social world in terms of the preschool children’s (Laible, Carlo,
Torquati and Ontai, 2004. Cited in Grusec and Davidov, 2010).
According to Parpal and Maccoby (1985), parent-child relationship
occurs not when the child is anxious but when parent and
child interact with each other as equal status partners (Grusec
and Davidov, 2010). Giving opportunities to children to control
the nature of interaction increases positive mood and behaviors
(Lay and at al., 1989; Parpal and Maccoby, 1985, Cited in Grusec
and Davidov, 2010). It was found that children having negative
relationship with parents more often displayed angry, aggressive,
hostile, disruptive behaviors in peer groups (Chen and et al.,
2005,Cited in Gülay, 2008).
Thus, the researcher believes that children with more consistent
child routines at home would have high level of social competence
for preschool children’s. In addition, based on existing evidence
of the relationship between child routines at home and preschool
children’s social competence in Western cultures [1, 5], we
predicted that more consistency in child routines at home would
be correlated to high level of social competence for preschool
children’s. Establishing the role of child routines at home in the
development of preschool children could inform strategies for
improving parent-child interactions and child social competence.
The current study was adopted Child Routines Questionnaire
(CRQ), which was originally developed by Sytsma et al. (2001)
[17] for school-aged children and later adapted by Wittig (2005)
[18] for use among preschool-aged children (CRQ-P). The CRQP
captures multiple aspects of child routines, such as daily living,
hygiene, social/educational activities, positive attention, and discipline
[1, 18]. The present study also adopted Social Competence
Teacher Rating Scale (SCTRS), this scales was develop and validated
by Girma Lemma., (Girma, 2014) [10]. The current study
used this scale to measure preschool children social competence
rated by preschool teachers. Reliability statistics for the overall instrument
and their subscales were checked. Thus, the mainobjective
of the current study was to examine the relationship between
child routines at home and social competence of preschool-aged
children in Addis Ababa, Ethiopia. The next objectives of this
study are to examine gender difference of child routines at home
and social competence of preschool children’s.
In general, it is believed that children’s competencies in the social
domain and child routines at home seem to be inadequately investigated
area. However, studies focus on child routines at home
would contribute in filling this gap. In preschools all children’s are
experiences the same child routines, but children’s are expertise’s
different child routines at home. That is why the current study
was home based where a child routines questionnaires was conducted
in schools, this information was gathered from parents
during school drop-off/pick-up or by going to the house of the
child that was reported for the child routines questionnaires. The
home environment, parental and teacher’s background, children
living arrangement, socioeconomic status and related home characteristics
are important determinants of children social competence.
In order to account for these variables, data was collected at
home characteristics of the child routines. The current study aims
to investigate the relationship between child routines at home and
social competence of preschool children’s in Addis Ababa.
Method
Research Design
The aim of this study was to examine the relationship between
child routines at home and social competence of preschool children’s
in Addis Ababa, Ethiopia. The study used correlational
research design to examine how child routines at home are correlated
with social competence of preschool children’s.
Participants
This study included 70 preschool children’s (age range 5 to 7
years) recruited through twogovernment preschools (Kibebe Tsehay and Menelik 2nd preschool) in Addis Ababa. In Ethiopia,
preschool programs are 3 years long and serve children aged from
4 to 6 years old. In this study, children were in their third year or
upper Kindergarten. Participating parents 40(60%) were mothers
and 28 (40%) were fathers. The parents reported children’s routines
at home, and preschool teachers rated the children’s social
competence.
Data Collection Instruments
Child Routines
We used the child routine questionnaire for preschooler (CRQ-P)
[18] to assess child routines. Parents reported the frequency of
daily routines on a three point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never)
to 3 (often) (see Table 1). The CRQ-P includes five subscales:
Discipline (five items), Daily Living (five items), Positive Attention
(five items), Education/Social (five items), and Hygiene (five
items). Adopted and conceptualize items related to child routines
at home to accommodate its use in Ethiopian context. One researcher
worked on the translating the child routine questionnaire
for preschool (CRQ-P) in Amharic. She has overseas educational
backgrounds and work assistant lecture in Addis Ababa University
in the field of psychology. For these translated the CRQ-P, my
thesis advisor provided feedback on the translation. Any inconsistencies
were discussed until a consensus was reached with my
thesis advisor, to ensure the accuracy and appropriateness of the
translation of questionnaire for Ethiopian context. Reliability of
the scale was determined by calculating coefficient alpha for the
25 items and each of the five-factors to measure internal consistency
of the final item-pool. Cronbach’s Alpha was .91 for the
CRQ: P Total Frequency scale. The coefficient alpha of the five
factors ranged from .85 to .72 [18].
Social Competence
The scales for Social Competence Teacher Rating Scale (SCTRS) was develop andvalidated by Girma Lemma., [10]. After getting permissionfrom the author, this study used the scale to measure preschool children social competence rated by preschools teachers. Reliability statistics for the overall instrument and their subscales were checked by the previous researcher and also checked by current study. The Cronbach alpha reliability for the 35 items is 0.87. At the subscale level, Cronbach alpha for pro-social behavior (12 items) was 0.84, for play skill (9 items) was 0.72, for emotional control (8 items) was 0.70, and for communication skill (6 items) was 0.81.
Procedures
The selected preschool principals have received a cooperation letter written by Addis Ababa University College of Education and Behavioral Studies, Center for Early Childhood Care and Education (ECCE), and then signed a permission note outlining of the study objective.
Data Analysis
In this study, quantitative and qualitative data analysis procedures were used. The quantitative data analysis used SPSS version 26. After careful data encoding and cleaning work, statistical analyses such as descriptive statistics,correlation, independent sample t-tests, ANOVA, and regression analysis were employed.
Ethical consideration
The researcher followed American Psychologist Association (APA) (2007) research ethical code. APA states that protect respondents’ privacy are first codes of ethics in human researches.
The researcher was being informed for respondents about the objectives of the study, purpose of the study, type of data collected, and the producers of data collection processes also explain to preschool principals, concerned professional, main and assistant teachers and parents in order to clarify expectations and reduce misconceptions.
Results
The Relationship between Child Routines at home and Social
Competence of preschool children’s
As the main objectives of the study, Pearson correlation coefficient
was computed to examine the relationship between child
routines at home and social competence of preschool children’s.
As indicated Table-3, all of the five domains in child routines at
home were positively correlated with their domains of social competence.
Pearson correlation coefficient result shows that child
routines in parent reported discipline routines were not statistically
significant and weak correlation to teacher reported social competence
domains; pro-social behavior (r= -.063, p= .605 > 0.05)
,play skills(r= .008, p= .897> 0.05), emotion regulation(r= .017,
p= .674>0.05), and communication skills(r= .081, p= .505>0.05).
Child routines in daily living were weak correlation and not statistically
significant correlation to teacher-reported children social
competences; pro-social behaviors (r= .004, p= .976), play skills(r
= .26, p=.036), emotion regulation(r=.12, p = .22), and communication
skills (r= .270, p= 0.024). Child routines in positive attention
were weak correlation and were not statistically significant
relationship to teacher reported social domains of children social
competence; pro-social behaviors(r= .12, p= .318), play skills(r=
.27, p = .16), emotion regulations(r= .11, p= .366), communication
skills (r=.27, p=.011). Child routines in education and social
weak correlation and were not statistically correlation to children
social competence domains; pro-social behavior(r = .05, p =.665),
play skills(r= .15, p= .523), emotion regulation(r= .21, p= .084),
and communication skills(r = .28, p= .022). Child routines in daily
hygiene activities were weak correlation and were not statistically
significant correlation to teachers reported children social competence
domains; pro-social behavior(r= .07, p=.66), play skills(r=
.16, p=.27), emotion regulation(r= .25, p= .08), and communication
skills(r =.22, p= .012).
Gender Difference on Social Competence
An independent sample t-test was computed to assess if there was
gender difference in social competence between boy and girl preschool
children. The result indicates that there was no statistically
significant difference between male and female for preschool children
in their social competence with males (pro-social behavior,
M=3.00, SD = .52) and females pro-social behavior (M = 2.98,
SD = .53); the degree of the mean difference =.02642, Sig = .835
and df= 68, play skills with male (M =2.45, SD = .48) and females
(M= 2.45, SD= .48); mean difference = 00272, sig= .981 and df=
68, emotion regulation with male(M =2.14, SD= .51 and female
(M= 2.06, SD= .45); the degree of the mean difference = .083,
Sig= .48, and df = 68, and the last sub-scale, communication skills
with male (M= 2.57, SD = .53) and with female(M=2.5392, SD=
.61963); the degree of the mean difference = .039, at Sig= .776,
and df= 68). These results indicated that between boy and girl
is not significant difference in social competence for preschool
children (see Table-5 below).
Table 3. The correlation between child routines at home and social competence of preschool children’s (N= 70).
Table 4. The total correlation between child routines at home and social competence of preschool children’s (N= 70).
Gender Difference of child routines at home in preschool children’s
An independent sample t-test was computed to assess if there was gender difference in child routines at home between male and female preschool children. The result indicates that there was no statistically significant but there is difference between male and female for preschool children in their child routines at home with males (discipline routines, M=1.42, SD = .40) and females discipline routines (M = 1.36, SD = .46); the degree of the mean difference =.058, Sig = .581 and df= 68, daily living routines with male (M =1.42, SD = .50) and females (M= 1.38, SD= .48); mean difference = .04, sig= .73 and df= 68, positive attention with male(M =1.03, SD= .47 and female (M= 1.09, SD= .44); the degree of the mean difference = .-055, Sig= .62, and df = 68, social and education related routines with male (M= 1.39, SD = .49) and with female(M=1.28, SD= .48); the degree of the mean difference = .11, at Sig= .33, and df= 68), and hygiene related routines with male( M = 1.51, SD = .58, with female M= 1.41, SD = .58 ) the degree of mean difference = .100, this result indicates that there is no mean difference rather child routines at home specifically hygiene related routines are equal unlike other routines. These results indicated that between male and female is not significant butthere difference in child routines at home for preschool children’s except hygiene related routines, hygiene related routines are equal for all children’s (see Table-6 below). Parent’s Educational Level and Children Social Competence One way (ANOVA) was employed to investigate social competence difference as a function of participants’ parental educational level. The findings indicated as there was no statistically significant difference among participants based on their educational level with (F= .41, p = .93) for children’s social competence.
Social Competence and Children Living Arrangement
One way (ANOVA) was employed to investigate social competence difference as a function of participants’ living arrangements. The findings indicated as there was no statistically significant difference among participants based on their living arrangement with (F= .99, p = .41) for children’s social competence. In sum, the findings about practices of parent’s child routines at home were examined; there wasn’t any statistically significant difference between mean scores that parental educational level and children living arrangement on child routines at home and social competence of preschool children’s.
Discussion
Along with the undeveloped economic and social changes in
Ethiopia, most of Ethiopian Parents are given minimum valuing
their child’s development for preschool age and strive to find
ways to enhance it. Providing children with structure and routines
canbe an effective strategy to promote social competence
and child well being. However, there is noempirical evidence has
been reported regarding the role of child routines at home and its
relationship with social competence of preschool children. The
current study provides initial evidence of the role that child routines
at home may play in strengthening social competence of
preschool children’s.
The current study produced two major findings. First, as expected,
children with a more routinized life experienced at home
higher levels of social competence. Possibly, when children have
a clear idea about what they are expected to perform at a certain
time during the day (i.e., routines indiscipline, daily living, education/
social, positive attention, and hygiene related activities), they
are more inclined to cooperate and comply. This may naturally
lead to fewer increased intimacy in parent-child relationships and
improve social competence of preschool children’s.
Furthermore, when children have formed consistent expectations
that howthey are treated depends on their specific behaviors (i.e.,
discipline routines) rather than random factors such as parents’
mood, they would be more confident that parents’ intention is to
discipline their behaviors instead of punishing them for reasons
over which they have no control. This perception of stability and
security may improve the quality of children’s social competence.
Second, child routines in the home context were also related to
children’s social competence, which is consistent with previous
findings regarding preschool children in Western societies [1, 5].
Activities involved in child routines at home require children’s
transition from one activity to another at a specified time and
need them to comply with the rules set by parents. Therefore,
child routines can provide chance for children to practice their
social relationship skills. This study also showed that the associations
between child routines and social competence of preschool
children were relatively weak. It is worth noting that in this
study, child routinesin home context were reported by parents,
and whereas child social competence of preschool children was
reported by teachers.
Overall, the present study supported the relationships between
child routines at home and social competence of preschool children’s.
Although previous evidence has shown the positive role
of child routines in child development, earlier studies did not
consider the confounding effect of child routines at home and
social competence of preschool children. The present study demonstrated
that child routines did make a unique contribution to
child development beyond social competence. This indicates that
child routines in themselves play a positive role in social competence
of preschool children, regardless of the child routines at
home adopted by parents. Furthermore, it is important to highlight
that it is of great value to demonstrate the positive associations
between child routines at home andsocial development in
Ethiopian preschool context. In Ethiopia, setting childroutines is
often more emphasized in preschool settings than in theat home
context. It appears that the importance of setting child routinesat
home has not been fully recognized by Ethiopian parents.
In addition, setting child routines for preschool children can be
challenging in Ethiopia, because in Ethiopia, grandparents and
significant others are often involved in their care and development
roles. Parents and grandparents often have different child-rearing
values, stress different socialization goals, and employ different
parenting strategies, which canhinder the setting of consistent
child routines for children. The findings ofthis study provide insight
for Ethiopian parents with preschool children, highlighting
that setting child routines at home could be an applicable programto
enhance their social relationships and competence with
their children while promoting the children’s social competence.
Thus, it is recommended that parents overcome the potential
challenges of daily hassles and provide structure and consistency
in their children’s daily lives.
Despite the contributions and implications described above, thisstudy
has limitations in two aspects. First, the present study is
correlational. Causal conclusion cannot be drawn regarding the
relationsbetween child routines at home and social competence
of preschool children’s. Longitudinal studies are needed to examinewhether
consistency in child routines at home can influence
social competence of preschool children’s. Finally, the whole parentsincluded
in the current sample had relatively low socio-economic
and education status. Thus, future research should examine
the relations between childroutines at home and social competence
of preschool children using experimental and longitudinal
designs, and test whether the findings are consistent among familiesfrom
higher socioeconomic and educational backgrounds in
Ethiopia.
Acknowledgements
We would like to express our sincere thanks to Addis Ababa University for providing free scholarship for my MA study and the
study participants for their cooperation.
Authors’ Contributions
YB contributed to study conception and design, data collection,
analysis and interpretation and drafting and revising the manuscript.
MH participated to the study design, data interpretation,
and review of the manuscript. Both authors participated in giving
final approval of the version to be published.
Author’s information
YB is Early Childhood Care and Development Specialist at Information
Network Security Administration (INSA). MH is also
a Social Psychology Researcher at Information Network Security
Agency (INSA).
References
- Bater LR, Jordan SS. Child routines and self-regulation serially mediate parenting practices and externalizing problems in preschool children. In Child & Youth Care Forum. Springer. 2017 Apr;46(2):243-259.
- Bierman KL. Peer rejection: Developmental processes and intervention strategies. Guilford Press; 2004.
- Bridley A, Jordan SS. Child routines moderate daily hassles and children's psychological adjustment. ChildHealth Care. 2012 Apr 30;41(2):129-44.
- Duncan GJ, Dowsett CJ, Claessens A, Magnuson K, Huston AC, Klebanov P, et al. School readiness and later achievement. Dev Psychol. 2007 Nov;43(6):1428-1446. PubMed PMID: 18020822.
- Ferretti LK, Bub KL. The influence of family routines on the resilience of low-income preschoolers. JAppl Dev Psychol. 2014 May 1;35(3):168-80.
- Fiese BH. Family routines and rituals. Yale University Press; 2006.
- Fiese BH. Routines of daily living and rituals in family life: A glimpse at stability and change during the early child-raising years. Zero to Three. 2002;22(4):10-13.
- Fiese BH, Tomcho TJ, Douglas M, Josephs K, Poltrock S, Baker T. A review of 50 years of research on naturally occurring family routines and rituals: cause for celebration? J Fam Psychol. 2002 Dec;16(4):381-90. PubMed PMID: 12561283.
- Geraldine F. A research paper on Children’s Early Learning and Development. NationalCouncil and Curriculum Development Association (NCCA). 2007.
- Lemma G. Quality of early childhood care and education in addisababa: Caregiver-child interaction, parental perception and social competence of children. Doctoral dissertation, Addis Ababa University, Addis Ababa. 2014.
- Guralnick MJ. Social competence and early intervention. J Early Interv. 1990 Jan;14(1):3-14.
- Katz LG, McClellan DE. Fostering children's social competence: The teacher's role. National Association for the Education of Young Children; 1997.
- Keltner B. Family characteristics of preschool social competence among black children in a Head Start Program. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1990 Winter;21(2):95-108. PubMed PMID: 2249498.
- Kliewer W, Kung E. Family moderators of the relation between hassles and behavior problems in inner-city youth. J Clin Child Psychol. 1998 Oct;27(3):278-92. PubMed PMID: 9789188.
- Howes C. Social competence with peers in young children: Developmental sequences. Dev Rev. 1987 Sep 1;7(3):252-72.
- Sytsma SE, Kelley ML, Wymer JH. Development and initial validation of the child routines Questionnaire. JPsychopathol and Behav Assess. 2001 Dec;23(4):241-51.
- Sytsma SE, Kelley ML, Wymer JH. Development and initial validation of the child routines inventory. JPsychopathol and Behav Assess. 2001 Dec;23(4):241-51.
- Wittig MM. Development and validation of the Child Routines Questionnaire: Preschool (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, Louisiana. 2005.